Selasa, 26 Februari 2013

Bahan Mata Kuliah Phonology


 1.    Linguistics adalah suatu bidang ilmu yang mengarah kepada study bahasa. Linguistics dapat di katakan sebagai aturan yang di gunakan dalam bahasa yang mengkaji semua wujud dan aturan-aturan dalam berbahasa, yang mana linguistics atau ilmu bahasa tersebut tidaklah tertumpu kepada satu bahasa saja, akan tetapi berbagai macam ragam seni berbahasa di dunia ini.

Language dapat di sebut sebagai alat komunikasi antara anggota masyarakat yang berupa symbol bunyi yang di hasilkan oleh alat ucap manusia dan system komunikasi yang mempergunakan symbol-symbol vocal ( bunyi ujaran ). Selain itu bahasa juga merupakan tanda yang jelas untuk menentukan kepribadian seseorang yang baik maupun yang buruk, tanda yang jelas untuk membedakan dai ras, suku, marga keluarga dan bangsa.

2.   Linguistics dapat di timbulkan melalui organ tubuh manusia terutama dalam jasmani. Adapun bagaimana kita dapat memahaminya terdapat pada proses berbicara dengan begitu kita dapat mengetahui maksud dari apa yang telah orang bicarakan secara jelas, kita dapat memahami dengan jelas kejadian dan fakta yang ada dengan kebenaran yang di jelaskan.

      Linguistics knowledge adalah pengtahuan dalam mempelajari bahasa yang mencakup tentang pengetahuan tata bahasa, symbol-symbol bahasa dan semua yang berhubungan dengan tata bahasa. Linguistics knowledge juga merupakan suatu gabungan dari kuantitas suatu ilmu tata bahasa dengan informasi yang di ketahui oleh seseorang yang aspek bahasanya tidak perlu di perhitungkan lagi dalam tata bahasa.
     
      Linguistics performance adalah ilmu bahasa yang mempelajari tentang bagaimana berbahasa dengan baik, sopan dan benar ketika berinteraksi dengan orang lain agar mendapatkan kesan yang baik.

3.   Language related to brain, istilah ini menyebutkan bahwa bahasa dan otak saling berkesinambungan. Kedua bagian ini sangatlah erat hubungannya dimana otak berperan penting terhadap perkembangan bahasa. Adapun cara bahasa masuk ke dalam otak meliputi berbagai proses, yakni bahasa masuk ke dalam otak di awali melalui proses pendengaran kemudian di rekam dan di simpan ke dalam otak yang di dalamnya  dip roses olewh organ yang namanya neuro untuk di sampaikan. Karena banyak bahasa yang dapat di hasilkan oleh otak, yang mana otak memikirkan suatu bahasa dan bahasa itu di keluarkan dalam bentuk suara oleh mulut maka bunyi yang di hasilkan oleh mulut inilah yang di sebut bahasa. Apabila terjadi kerusakan dalam otak maka bahasa akan terbatas karena otak tidak mengahasilkan atau tidak banyak memproduksi bahasa.

4.   Grammatically adalah himpunan structural aturan yang mengatur susunan kalimat, frasa dan kata-kata yang di berikan secara alami. Grammatically juga biasa di gunakan dalam pembetulan atau penjelasan maksud dan tujuan dalam suatu bahasa tentang makna yang terkandung di dalam penyampaiannya.

5.   Lexion adalah koleksi leksem pada suatu bahasa, istilah ini berasal dari bahasa yunani yang kurang lebih bermakna ‘perihal kata’. Lexion juga di sebut sebagai komponen bahasa yang memuat semua informasi tentang makna dan pemakaian kata dalam bahasa, singkatnya lexion adalah suatu bahasa dengan kosa kata termasuk di dalamnya kata-kata dan ekspresi.

6.  Morphemes adalah bagian terkecil dari kata yang mana bagian terkewcil itu memiliki makna atau arti. Morphemes juga menjelaskan tentang bagian terkecil dalam bahasa yang dapat di bagi dalam 2 bunyi.
     Contoh: love – loved
        Run – runs


     Words merupakan unsure utama pembentuk phrasa, klousa dan kalimat. Terdapat dua unsure utama dalam kata yaitu kata dasar dan berimbuhan (akhiran, awalan atau sisipan)
     Contoh: Tangis (kata)
       Menangis (berimbuhan)

      Prase merupakan beberapa kata yang mengandung arti tertentu atau di sebut juga suatu ucapan/ungkapan yang di sampaikan kepada seseorang untuk memenuhi suatu keinginan yang di maksud.
     Contoh: buku itu bagus
        komputer baru

Sentences adalah gabungan dari beberapa kata yang memiliki makna tertentu. Sentences terdiri dari beberapa kata yang di gabungkan dan mempunyai kaitan antara satu kata dengan kata lainnya, dan terdiri dari subjek dan predikat.
     Contoh: Monika sedang memasak di dapur.
        Ayah sedang bekerja di sawah.

      7.  homophones – orthopist
Kata yaqng sama lafalnya dengan kata lain tetapi berbeda ejaan dan         maknanya.
      Contoh: Masa dan massa
         Sangsi dan sanksi
         Bisa (racun) dan Bisa (mampu)

Orthography – hyponyms
      Adalah system ejaan suatu bahasa atau gambaran bunyi bahasa yang berupa tulisan atau lambang.
Contoh: Negara=staet=stat
         Danau=laek=lak
         Tikus=mouse=mous

Entailment – contradiction
Adalah hubungan antara satu kalimat dan sebuah kalimat lainnya tetapi    memiliki makna yang berbeda.
      Contoh: Bejalan><tidak berlari
         Duduk><berdiri
         Ramai><sepi

Metaphons – idioms
Merupakan cara sistematis seperti kata tunggal yang yang tidak hanya mempunyai satu fungsi.
Contoh: Kick the bucked
        Fly off the handle
        Spill the beans

      8.  Phonology adalah bagian dari ilmu bahasa yang mempelajari tata     bunyi/kaidah bunyi dan cara menghasilkan. Phonology merupakan bagian penting dari suara atau bunyi di dalam pengucapan bahasa.

Phonetics adalah hasil berupa suara atau bunyi yang di hasilkan oleh huruf yang kita ucapkan sebagai pembeda fungsi makna yangt kita maksud.

Perbedaannya adalah dimana phonology mempelajari tentang pengucapan bunyi sedangkan phonetics adalah sebagai hasil dari pengucapan bunyi yang berbentuk suara dan di gunakan sebagai alat komunikasi.

9.   Rules off phonology
-          Phonetic
-          Alat ucap
-          Proses fonasi
-          Klasifikasi bahasa
-          Klasifikasi vocal
-          Silabel

        Fungsi phonology
1.    Untuk mengetahui perubahan bunyi suara pada suatu kata.
2.    Mempelajari cara kerja organ tubuh manusia terutama yang berhubungan dengan penggunaan bahasa.
3.    Mempelajari tentang proses pembentukan bunyi, penyampaian/ pengucapannya.

10.   Language change merupakan penomena yang berkenaan dengan ilmu phonetics, morphological, sematic, syintatic dan corak bahasa lainnya yang berganti dari wktu ke waktu, di mana bahasa itu berkembang dan berubah sesuai dengan perkembangan zaman itu sendiri dan perubahan atau pergantian itu sendiri terjadi pada bunyi bahasa tersebut, hal ini di sebabkan oleh perbedaan dan kemajuan dalam pengucapan bahasa karena bahasa itu berkembang dan berubah mengikuti perkembangan zaman.













Phonology

Phonology adalah ilmu yang mempelajari tentang suara. Ilmi ini sangat penting karena phonology dalam bahasa inggris sangatlah mirip.
https://blogger.googleusercontent.com/img/b/R29vZ2xl/AVvXsEhLbrNkuBuLF3PVpwxTvKejBi25ROBBUO_4oedab_67rPMGPbF1jqUF5Q43MQNBgFyJfhI_YkeFyQClmxXffb8XjzJvU5Dvo30jho8VvkxDrrXWmDpAVklV-zvVVx2qcpWBkbtEFXrz/s320/4.jpg


NB :

Laring : pipa tempat beradanya pita suara yang akan menuju paru-paruh
Paring : pipa diantara mulut yang bisa membuka dan menutup sesuai dengan kebutuhan pipa
Nassal : suara yang keluar dari hidung / hidung
Vocal track : smua rangkaian pembentuk suara mulai dari hidung sampai pipa yang ada di paru-paru
Phonetic : ilmu umum yang mempelajari karakteristik suara dibicarakan/disuarakan
Voiceless sound : suara yang terbentuk ketika vocal cords melebar kemudian udara dari tengorokan melewati diantaranya

Voice sound : suara yang terbentuk ketika vocal cords tertarik bersama kemudian udara dari tenggorokan berulang-ulang menekannya membuat efek getaran,

Tempat artikulasi ada 7 dalam B.inggris :

1.       Bilabials : menggunakan bibir contoh B , P
2.       Labiodentals : bibir bawah dan gigi contoh v, f
3.       Dental : menggunakan gigi , contoh : 0 seret tengah ( bath ) voiceless
4.       Alveolars : menggunakan lidah dan gusi, contoh : t,d,s,z
5.       Palatals : menggunakan langit-langit, contoh : “sh” pada shoot
6.       Velars : menggunakan lebih kebelakang / mundur dari langit-langit, contoh : k,g
7.       Glottals  menggunakan ruang dan tanpa menggunakan lidah ataupun bibir

Cara mengartikulasi ada 6 dalam B.inggris :

1.       Stop : stop airstream then letting it go abruptly. Example : ten ; voiceless alveoral stop
2.       Fricatives : combination of a brief stopping of the airstream with an obstructedrelease which causes some friction
3.       Nasals : the velum is lowered and the airstream is allowed to flow out through the nose to produce [m], [n]
4.       Liquids : formed by letting the airstream flow around the sides of the tongue as the tip of the tongue makes contact with the middle of the alveolar ridge. Example : L, R
5.       Glides ( menggilinding ) : produced with the tongue in motion to or from the position of a vowel and sometimes are semi-vowel. Example : W,J Bo”y”
6.       Vowel : produced with a relatively free flow of air

Perbedaan antara vowel sound dan consonant sound

Vowel : tongue, lack of construction, more soundneress, nucleous
Consonant : 7 place of articulation, lack of construction, lack of soundneress, not nucleous
Diphthongs : a kind of vowel which show a change of quality in singel syllabel (suku kata ) contoh :  buy (aj), cow (aw), boy (oj) boy


Bahan Mata Kuliah Phonology


English phonology is the sound system (phonology) of the English language, or to the study of that system. Like many languages, English has wide variation in pronunciation, both historically and from dialect to dialect. In general, however, the regional dialects of English share a largely similar (though not identical) phonological system.
Phonological analysis of English often concentrates on, or uses as a reference point, one or more of the prestige or standard accents, such as Received Pronunciation for England,General American for the United States, and General Australian for Australia.
Contents
  [hide
·         1 Phonemes
o    1.2 Vowels
·         2 Lexical stress
·         3 Phonotactics
·         4 Prosody
o    4.2 Rhythm
·         6 Dialectal differences
·         7 See also
·         8 References
·         9 Bibliography
·         10 External links
[edit]Phonemes
phoneme of a language or dialect is an abstraction of a speech sound or of a group of different sounds which are all perceived to have the same function by speakers of that language or dialect. For example, the English word "through" consists of three phonemes: the initial "th" sound, the "r" sound, and an "oo" vowel sound. Notice that the phonemes in this and many other English words do not always correspond directly to the letters used to spell them (English orthography is not as strongly phonemic as that of certain other languages).
The phonemes of English and their number vary from dialect to dialect, and also depend on the interpretation of the individual researcher. The number of consonant phonemes is generally put at 24 (or slightly more). The number of vowels is subject to greater variation; in the system presented on this page there are 20 vowel phonemes in Received Pronunciation, 14–16 in General American and 20–21 in Australian English. The pronunciation keys used in dictionaries generally contain a slightly greater number of symbols than this, to take account of certain sounds used in foreign words and certain noticeable distinctions that may not be strictly speaking phonemic.
[edit]Consonants
The following table shows the 24 consonant phonemes found in most dialects of English. When consonants appear in pairs, fortis consonants (i.e., aspirated or voiceless) appear on the left and lenis consonants (i.e., lightly voiced or voiced) appear on the right:



Consonant phonemes of English

m
n
ŋ
p  b
t  d
k  ɡ
tʃ  dʒ
f  v
θ  ð
s  z
ʃ  ʒ
(x)2
h
r1, 5
j
w3
l1
1.     Most varieties of English have syllabic consonants, for example at the end of bottle and button. In such cases, no vowel is pronounced between the last two consonants. It is common for syllabic consonants to be transcribed with a subscript mark, so that phonetic transcription of bottle would be [ˈbɒtl̩] and for button [ˈbʌtn̩]. In theory, such consonants could be analysed as individual phonemes. However, this would add several extra consonant phonemes to the inventory for English,[1] and phonologists prefer to identify syllabic nasals and liquids phonemically as /əC/.[2][3] Thus button is phonemically /ˈbʌtən/ and 'bottle' is phonemically /ˈbɒtəl/.
2.     The voiceless velar fricative /x/ is mainly restricted to Scottish English; words with /x/ in Scottish accents tend to be pronounced with /k/ in other dialects. The velar fricative may appear in recently borrowed words such as chutzpah.
3.     The sound at the beginning of words spelt wh (e.g. whichwhy) is in some accents (e.g. much of the American South, Scotland, and Ireland) a "voiceless w" sound, which is a voiceless labiovelar fricative[4][5][6] or voiceless labiovelar approximant,[7] whereas other accents have the voiced approximant [w]. The phonemic status of the voiceless sound, for which the phonetic symbol is [ʍ], is difficult to define. It would be possible to consider this sound to be a separate phoneme, but phonologists prefer to treat it as a combination of /h/ and /w/. Thus which (as pronounced by speakers who have the "voiceless w") is transcribed phonemically as /hwɪtʃ/. This should not, however, be interpreted to mean that such speakers actually pronounce [h] followed by [w]: the phonemic transcription /hw/ is simply a convenient way of representing a single sound[ʍ] without analyzing such dialects as having an extra phoneme.[8]
4.     A similar case to the above is that of the sound at the beginning of huge; in accents in which the initial consonant is voiceless, a voiceless palatal fricative [ç] occurs, but the usual phonemic analysis is to treat this as /h/ plus /j/ so that huge is transcribed /hjuːdʒ/. This transcription often gives rise to the incorrect belief that speakers pronounce [h] followed by [j] in such contexts, but the symbols in fact represent a single sound [ç].[8] The yod-dropping found in Norfolk dialect means that the traditional Norfolk pronunciation of huge is [hʊudʒ] and not [çuːdʒ].
5.     The phonotactic constraints regarding the phoneme /r/ differ among accents. In non-rhotic accents, such as Received Pronunciation and Australian English, /r/ only appears before a vowel, whereas in rhotic accents /r/ occurs in all positions.
The following table shows typical examples of the occurrence of the above consonant phonemes in words.
/p/
pit
/b/
bit
/t/
tin
/d/
din
/k/
cut
/ɡ/
gut
/tʃ/
cheap
/dʒ/
jeep
/f/
fat
/v/
vat
/θ/
thin
/ð/
then
/s/
sap
/z/
zap
/ʃ/
she
/ʒ/
measure
/x/
loch
/w/
we
/m/
map
/l/
left
/n/
nap
/r/
run
/j/
yes
/h/
ham
/ŋ/
bang
The distinctions between the nasals are neutralized in some environments. For example, before a final /p/, /t/ or /k/ there is only one nasal sound that can appear in each case:[m], [n] or [ŋ] respectively (as in the words limplintlink – note that the n of link is pronounced [ŋ]). This effect can even occur across syllable or word boundaries, particularly in stressed syllables: synchrony is pronounced as [ˈsɪŋkɹəni] whereas synchronic may be pronounced either as [sɪŋˈkɹɒnɨk] or as [sɪnˈkɹɒnɨk]. For other possible syllable-final combinations, see Coda in the Phonotactics section below.


[edit]Allophones of consonants
An allophone is one of a set of multiple possible spoken sounds (or phones) used to pronounce a single phoneme. For example, the phoneme /t/ is pronounced differently in tonsilsthan in button, and still differently in cat. All of these "t" sounds are allophones of the same phoneme, since no two words can be distinguished from each other solely on the basis of which of these pronunciations is used.
Although regional variation is very great across English dialects, certain instances of allophony can be observed in all (or at least the vast majority) of English accents. (See alsoAllophones of vowels below.)
·         Many dialects have two allophones of /l/ – the "clear" L and the "dark" or velarized L. The clear variant is used before vowels (or sometimes only before stressed vowels), the dark variant in other positions. In some dialects, /l/ may be always clear (e.g. Wales, Ireland, the Caribbean) or always dark (e.g. Scotland, most of North America, Australia, New Zealand).
·         Depending on dialect, /r/ has at least the following allophones in varieties of English around the world:
·         alveolar approximant [ɹ]
·         postalveolar or retroflex approximant [ɻ]
·         labiodental approximant [ʋ]
·         alveolar tap [ɾ]
·         post-alveolar flap [ɽ]
·         alveolar trill [r]
In the traditional Tyneside accent in the North of England, /r/ was pronounced as a voiced uvular fricative [ʁ], but this is probably now extinct.[9]
In some rhotic accents, such as General American, /r/ when not followed by a vowel is realized as an r-coloring of the preceding vowel or its coda.
For many speakers, /r/ is somewhat labialized, as in reed [ɹʷiːd] and tree [tʰɹ̥ʷiː]. In the latter case, the [t] may be slightly labialized as well.[10]
·         Postalveolar consonants are also usually labialized (e.g. /ʃ/ is pronounced [ʃʷ] and /ʒ/ is pronounced [ʒʷ]).
·         The voiceless stops /p/, /t/ and /k are aspirated ([pʰ], [tʰ], [kʰ]) at the beginnings of words (for example tomato) and at the beginnings of word-internal stressed syllables (for example potato). They are unaspirated ([p], [t], [k]) after /s/ (stan, span, scan) and at the ends of syllables.[11]
·         In American English, both /t/ and /d/ can be pronounced as a voiced flap [ɾ] in certain positions: when they come between a preceding stressed vowel (possibly with intervening/r/) and precede an unstressed vowel or syllabic L. Examples include waterbottlepetalpeddle (the last two words sound alike). The flap may even appear at word boundaries, as in put it on. When the combination /nt/ appears in such positions, some American speakers pronounce it as a nasalized flap that may become indistinguishable from /n/, so winter may be pronounced as similar or identical to winner.[12]
·         In many accents of English, voiceless stops (/p/, /t/, /k/ and /tʃ/ are glottalized. This may be heard either as a glottal stop preceding the oral closure ("pre-glottalization" or "glottal reinforcement") or as a substitution of the glottal stop [ʔ] for the oral stop (glottal replacement). Pre-glottalization normally occurs in British and American English when the voiceless consonant phoneme is followed by another consonant or when the consonant is in final position. Thus football and catching are often pronounced [ˈfʊʔtbɔːl] and[ˈʔtʃɪŋ], respectively. Glottal replacement often happens in cases such as those just given, so that football is frequently pronounced [ˈfʊʔbɔːl]. In addition, however, glottal replacement is increasingly common in British English when /t/ occurs between vowels if the preceding vowel is stressed; thus getting better is often pronounced by younger speakers as [ˈɡeʔɪŋ ˌbeʔə].[13]
·         Final /t/ as in cat is not usually audibly released. However, in speech with careful enunciation, in all situations /t/ may be pronounced as [t] or [tʰ].
The foregoing features mean that English voiceless plosive consonants have a wide range of different allophones.[14]:pp.62-67)
[edit]Vowels
The vowels of English differ considerably between dialects. Because of this, corresponding vowels may be transcribed with various symbols depending on the dialect under consideration. When considering English as a whole, lexical sets are often used, each named by a word containing the vowel or vowels in question. For example, the LOT set consists of words which, like lot, have /ɒ/ in Received Pronunciation and /ɑ/ in General American. The "LOT vowel" then refers to the vowel that appears in those words in whichever dialect is being considered, or (at a greater level of abstraction) to a diaphoneme which transcends all dialects. A commonly used system of lexical sets is presented below; for each set, the corresponding phonemes are given for RP (first column) and General American (second column), using the notation that will be used on this page.
TRAP
BATH
PALM
LOT
CLOTH
THOUGHT
KIT
FLEECE
i
DRESS
e
STRUT
FOOT
GOOSE
u
FACE
eɪ
PRICE
aɪ
CHOICE
ɔɪ
GOAT
əʊ
oʊ
MOUTH
aʊ
NURSE
ɜː(r)
ɜr
START
ɑː(r)
ɑr
NORTH
ɔː(r)
ɔr
FORCE
ɔː(r)
ɔr, oʊr
NEAR
ɪə(r)
ɪr
SQUARE
eə(r)
ɛr
CURE
ʊə(r)
ʊr
COMMA
LETTER
ər
HAPPY
iːɪ
i
For a table that shows the pronunciations of these vowels in a wider range of English dialects, see IPA chart for English dialects.
The following tables show the vowel phonemes of three standard varieties of English. The notation system used here for Received Pronunciation (RP) is fairly standard; the others less so. For different ways of transcribing General American, see Transcription variants below. The feature descriptions given here (front, close, etc.) are abstracted somewhat; the actual pronunciations of these vowels are more accurately conveyed by the IPA symbols used (see Vowel for a chart indicating the meanings of these symbols; though note also the points listed below the following tables).
Received Pronunciation[15]

long
short
long
short
long
short


e


eɪ   aɪ   ɔɪ   aʊ   əʊ
ɪə   eə   ʊə

General American

long
short
long
short
long
short


(ɜː)

(ʌ)

eɪ   aɪ   ɔɪ   aʊ   oʊ
(
ɪə)   (eə)

Australian English

long
short
long
short
long
short


e

a

æɪ   ɑɪ   oɪ   æɔ   əʉ
ɪə   ʉə

The differences between these tables can be explained as follows:
1.      The absence of length marks in the General American table is largely a matter of notational convention.
2.      In General American, the vowels [ə], [ʌ] and [ɜ] may be considered a single phoneme.
3.      General American lacks a phoneme corresponding to RP /ɒ/ (LOT, CLOTH), instead using /ɑ/ or /ɔ/ in such words.
4.      General American does not have the centering diphthong phonemes /ɪə/, /ɛə/, and /ʊə/; in NEAR, SQUARE, and CURE it has the combinations /ɪr/, /ɛr/, /ʊr/. (However in some descriptions these words are analyzed as diphthongs even in rhotic dialects.[16][page needed])
5.      In certain General American dialects, the diphthongs /ɪə/ and /eə/ can be found in words such as "ideas" and "rail," respectively.
6.      The different notations used for the vowel of GOAT in RP and General American (/əʊ/ and /oʊ/) reflect a difference in the most common phonetic realizations of that vowel.
7.      The different notations used here for some of the Australian vowels reflect the phonetic realization of those vowels in Australian: a central [ʉː] rather than [uː] in GOOSE, a more closed [e] rather than [ɛ] in DRESS, an open-mid [ɔ] rather than RP's [ɒ] in LOT and CLOTH, a more close [oː] rather than [ɔː] in THOUGHT, NORTH and FORCE, a fronted [a]rather than [ʌ] in STRUT, a fronted [aː] rather than [ɑː] in CALM and START, and somewhat different pronunciations of most of the diphthongs.
8.      The Australian monophthong /eː/ corresponds to the RP diphthong /ɛə/ (SQUARE).
9.      Australian has the bad–lad split, with distinctive short and long variants of [æ] in various words of the TRAP set.
10.  The vowel /ʊə/ is often omitted from descriptions of Australian, as for most speakers it has split into the long monophthong /oː/ (e.g. poorsure) or the sequence /ʉː.ə/(e.g. curelure).[17]
Other points to be noted are these:
·         Although the notation /ʌ/ is used for the vowel of STRUT in RP, the actual pronunciation is closer to a near-open central vowel [ɐ]. The symbol ʌ continues to be used for reasons of tradition (it was historically a back vowel) and because it is still back in other varieties.[18]
·         A significant number of words (the BATH group) have /æ/ in General American, but /ɑː/ in RP (and mostly /aː/ in Australian).
·         Many American speakers do not distinguish /ɑ/ from /ɔ/ (see cot–caught merger).
·         In General American (which is a rhotic accent – /r/ can occur in positions where it does not precede a vowel), many of the vowels can be r-colored by way of realization of a following /r/. This is often transcribed phonetically using a vowel symbol with an added retroflexion diacritic [˞]; thus the symbol [ɚ] has been created for an r-colored schwa(sometimes called "schwar") as in LETTER, and the vowel of START can be modified to make [ɑ˞] so that the word 'start' may be transcribed [stɑ˞t]. Alternatively, the START vowel might be written [stɑɚt] to indicate an r-colored offglide. The vowel /ɜ/ (as in NURSE) is generally always r-colored, and this can be written [ɝ] (or as a syllabic [ɹ̩]).
·         In RP and other dialects, many words from the CURE group are coming to be pronounced by an increasing number of speakers with the NORTH vowel (so sure is often pronounced like shore). Also the RP vowels /ɛə/ and /ʊə/ may be monophthongized to [ɛː] and [oː] respectively.[19]
·         Long vowels are often not pronounced as pure monophthongs. In particular, the vowels of FLEECE and GOOSE are usually pronounced as narrow diphthongs: [ɪi], [ʊu].
[edit]Allophones of vowels
Listed here are some of the significant cases of allophony of vowels found within standard English dialects (see also Allophones of consonants above).
·         There is a tendency for many vowels to be pronounced with greater length in open syllables than closed syllables, and with greater length in syllables ending with a voiced consonant than with a voiceless one. For example, the /aɪ/ in advise is longer than that in advice.
·         In many accents of English, tense vowels undergo breaking before /l/, resulting in pronunciations like [piəɫ] for peel, [puəɫ] for pool, [peəɫ] for pail, and [poəɫ] for pole.
·         In RP, the vowel /əʊ/ may be pronounced more back, as [oʊ], before /l/, as in goal. In Australian English the vowel /əʉ/ is similarly backed to [oʊ] before /l/.
·         The vowel /aɪ/ may be pronounced less open before a voiceless consonant.[14]:p.66 Thus writer may be distinguished from rider even when flapping causes the /t/ and /d/ to be pronounced identically.
·         The vowel /ə/ is often pronounced [ɐ] in open syllables.[20]
[edit]Unstressed syllables
For more information, see Stress and vowel reduction in English.
Unstressed syllables in English may contain almost any vowel, but there are certain sounds – characterized by central position and weakness – that are particularly often found as the nuclei of syllables of this type. These include:
·         schwa, [ə], as in COMMA and (in non-rhotic dialects) LETTER; also in many other positions such as aboutphotographpaddock, etc. This sound is essentially restricted to unstressed syllables exclusively. In the approach presented here it is identified with the phoneme /ə/, although other analyses do not have a separate phoneme for schwa and regard it as a reduction or neutralization of other vowels in syllables with the lowest degree of stress.
·         r-colored schwa, [ɚ], as in LETTER in General American and some other rhotic dialects, which can be identified with the underlying sequence /ər/.
·         syllabic consonants: [l̩] as in bottle, [n̩] as in button, [m̩] as in rhythm. These may be phonemized either as a plain consonant or as a schwa followed by a consonant; for example button may be represented as /ˈbʌtn/ or /ˈbʌtən/.
·         [ɪ], as in rosesmakingexpect. This can be identified with the phoneme /ɪ/, although in unstressed syllables it may be pronounced more centrally (in American tradition thebarred i symbol /ɨ/ is used here), and for some speakers (particularly in Australian and New Zealand and some American English) it is merged with /ə/ in these syllables. Among speakers who retain the distinction there are many cases where free variation between /ɪ/ and /ə/ is found, as in the second syllable of typical. (The OED has recently adopted the symbol /ᵻ/ to indicate such cases.)
·         [ʊ], as in argumenttoday, for which similar considerations apply as in the case of [ɪ]. (The symbol /ᵿ/ is sometimes used in these cases, similarly to /ᵻ/.) Some speakers may also have a rounded schwa, [ɵ], used in words like omission [ɵˈmɪʃən].[21]
·         [i], as in happycoffee, in many dialects (others have [ɪ] in this position).[22] The phonemic status of this [i] is not easy to establish. Contemporary accounts regard it as a symbol representing a close front vowel that is neither the vowel of KIT nor that of FLEECE; it occurs in contexts where the contrast between these vowels is neutralized.[23][24][25]Strictly speaking, therefore, [i] is not a phoneme but an archiphoneme. See happy-tensing.
·         [u], as in influenceto each. This is the back rounded counterpart to [i] described above; its phonemic status is treated in the same works as cited there.
Vowel reduction in unstressed syllables is a significant feature of English. Syllables of the types listed above often correspond to a syllable containing a different vowel ("full vowel") used in other forms of the same morpheme where that syllable is stressed. For example, the first o in photograph, being stressed, is pronounced with the GOAT vowel, but inphotography, where it is unstressed, it is reduced to schwa. Also, certain common words (aanoffor, etc.) are pronounced with a schwa when they are unstressed, although they have different vowels when they are in a stressed position (see Weak and strong forms in English).
Some unstressed syllables, however, retain full (unreduced) vowels, i.e. vowels other than those listed above. Examples are the /æ/ in ambition and the /aɪ/ in finite. Some phonologists regard such syllables as not being fully unstressed (they may describe them as having tertiary stress); some dictionaries have marked such syllables as havingsecondary stress. However linguists such as Ladefoged[26] and Bolinger[21] regard this as a difference purely of vowel quality and not of stress,[27] and thus argue that vowel reduction itself is phonemic in English. Examples of words where vowel reduction seems to be distinctive for some speakers[28] include chickaree vs. chicory (the latter has the reduced vowel of HAPPY, whereas the former has the FLEECE vowel without reduction), and Pharaoh vs. farrow (both have the GOAT vowel, but in the latter word it may reduce to [ɵ]).
[edit]Transcription variants
The choice of which symbols to use for phonemic transcriptions may reveal theoretical assumptions or claims on the part of the transcriber. English "lax" and "tense" vowels are distinguished by a synergy of features, such as heightlength, and contour (monophthong vs. diphthong); different traditions in the linguistic literature emphasize different features. For example, if the primary feature is thought to be vowel height, then the non-reduced vowels of General American English may be represented according to the table to the left and below. If, on the other hand, vowel length is considered to be the deciding factor, the symbols in the table to the below and center may be chosen (this convention has sometimes been used because the publisher did not have IPA fonts available, though that is seldom an issue any longer.) The rightmost table lists the corresponding lexical sets.
General American full vowels,
vowel height distinctive
i
u
ɪ
ʊ
e
ɚ
o
ɛ
ʌ
ɔ
æ
ɑ
General American full vowels,
vowel length distinctive
i
u
ɹ̩ː
e
ʌ
o
a
Lexical sets representing
General American full vowels
FLEECE
GOOSE
KIT
FOOT
FACE
NURSE
GOAT
DRESS
STRUT
THOUGHT
TRAP
PALM
If vowel transition is taken to be paramount, then the chart may look like one of these:
General American full vowels,
vowel contour distinctive
ij
uw
i
u
ej
ər
ow
e
ə
o
æ
ɑ
or
General American full vowels,
vowel contour distinctive
ɪi̯
ʊu̯
ɪ
ʊ
ɛɪ̯
ɝɹ
ɔʊ̯
ɛ
ʌ
ɔ
æ
ɑ
(The transcriber at left assumes that there is no phonemic distinction between semivowels and approximants, so that /ej/ is equivalent to /eɪ̯/.)
Many linguists combine more than one of these features in their transcriptions, suggesting they consider the phonemic differences to be more complex than a single feature.
General American full vowels,
height & length distinctive
ɪ
ʊ
ɝː
ɛ
ʌ
ɔ
æ
ɑː
or
General American full vowels,
height & contour distinctive
ij
uw
ɪ
ʊ
ej
ɜr
ow
ɛ
ʌ
ɔ
æ
ɑ
[edit]Lexical stress
Lexical stress is phonemic in English. For example, the noun increase and the verb increase are distinguished by the positioning of the stress on the first syllable in the former, and on the second syllable in the latter. (See initial-stress-derived noun.) Stressed syllables in English are louder than non-stressed syllables, as well as being longer and having a higher pitch.
In traditional approaches, in any English word consisting of more than one syllable, each syllable is ascribed one of three degrees of stress: primarysecondary or unstressed. Ordinarily, in each such word there will be exactly one syllable with primary stress, possibly one syllable having secondary stress, and the remainder unstressed. For example, the word amazing has primary stress on the second syllable, while the first and third syllables are unstressed, whereas the word organization has primary stress on the fourth syllable, secondary stress on the first, and the second, third and fifth unstressed. This is often shown in pronunciation keys using the IPA symbols for primary and secondary stress (which are ˈ and ˌ respectively), placed before the syllables to which they apply. The two words just given may therefore be represented (in RP) as /əˈmeɪzɪŋ/ and /ˌɔːɡənaɪˈzeɪʃən/.
Some analysts identify an additional level of stress (tertiary stress). This is generally ascribed to syllables that are pronounced with less force than those with secondary stress, but nonetheless contain a "full" or "unreduced" vowel (vowels that are considered to be reduced are listed under Vowels in unstressed syllables above). Hence the third syllable oforganization, if pronounced with /aɪ/ as shown above (rather than being reduced to /ɪ/ or /ə/), might be said to have tertiary stress. (The precise identification of secondary and tertiary stress differs between analyses; dictionaries do not generally show tertiary stress, although some have taken the approach of marking all syllables with unreduced vowels as having at least secondary stress.)
In some analyses, then, the concept of lexical stress may become conflated with that of vowel reduction. An approach which attempts to separate these two is provided by Peter Ladefoged, who states that it is possible to describe English with only one degree of stress, as long as unstressed syllables are phonemically distinguished for vowel reduction.[29]In this approach, the distinction between primary and secondary stress is regarded as a phonemic or prosodic detail rather than a phonemic feature – primary stress is seen as an example of the predictable "tonic" stress that falls on the final stressed syllable of a prosodic unit. For more details of this analysis, see Stress and vowel reduction in English.
For stress as a prosodic feature (emphasis of particular words within utterances), see Prosodic stress below.
[edit]Phonotactics
Phonotactics is the study of the sequences of phonemes that occur in languages and the sound structures that they form. In this study it is usual to represent consonants in general with the letter C and vowels with the letter V, so that a syllable such as 'be' is described as having CV structure. The IPA symbol used to show a division between syllables is the dot [.]. Syllabification is the process of dividing continuous speech into discrete syllables, a process in which the position of a syllable division is not always easy to decide upon.
Most languages of the world syllabify CVCV and CVCCV sequences as /CV.CV/ and /CVC.CV/ or /CV.CCV/, with consonants preferentially acting as the onset of a syllable containing the following vowel. According to one view, English is unusual in this regard, in that stressed syllables attract following consonants, so that ˈCVCV and ˈCVCCV syllabify as /ˈCVC.V/ and /ˈCVCC.V/, as long as the consonant cluster CC is a possible syllable coda; in addition, /r/ preferentially syllabifies with the preceding vowel even when both syllables are unstressed, so that CVrV occurs as /CVr.V/. This is the analysis used in the Longman Pronunciation Dictionary.[30] However, this view is not widely accepted, as explained in the following section.
[edit]Syllable structure
The syllable structure in English is (C)3V(C)5, with a near maximal example being strengths (/strɛŋkθs/, although it can be pronounced /strɛŋθs/).[31] From the phonetic point of view, the analysis of syllable structures is a complex task: because of widespread occurrences of articulatory overlap, English speakers rarely produce an audible release of individual consonants in consonant clusters.[32] This coarticulation can lead to articulatory gestures that seem very much like deletions or complete assimilations. For example,hundred pounds may sound like [ˈhʌndɹɪb pʰaʊndz] and 'jumped back' (in slow speech, [ˈdʒʌmptbæk]) may sound like [ˈdʒʌmpbæk], but X-ray[33] and electropalatographic[34][35][36] studies demonstrate that inaudible and possibly weakened contacts or lingual gestures may still be made. Thus the second /d/ in hundred pounds does not entirely assimilate to a labial place of articulation, rather the labial gesture co-occurs with the alveolar one; the "missing" [t] in 'jumped back' may still be articulated, though not heard.
Division into syllables is a difficult area, and different theories have been proposed. A widely accepted approach is the maximal onsets principle:[37] this states that, subject to certain constraints, any consonants in between vowels should be assigned to the following syllable. Thus the word 'leaving' should be divided /ˈliː.vɪŋ/ rather than */ˈliːv.ɪŋ/, and 'hasty' is /ˈheɪ.sti/ rather than */ˈheɪs.ti/ or */ˈheɪst.i/. However, when such a division results in an onset cluster which is not allowed in English, the division must respect this. Thus if the word 'extra' were divided */ˈɛ.kstrə/ the resulting onset of the second syllable would be /kstr/, a cluster which does not occur in English. The division /ˈɛk.strə/ is therefore preferred. If assigning a consonant or consonants to the following syllable would result in the preceding syllable ending in an unreduced short vowel, this is avoided. Thus the word 'comma' should be divided /ˈkɒm.ə/ and not */ˈkɒ.mə/, even though the latter division gives the maximal onset to the following syllable, because English syllables do not end in /ɒ/.
In some cases, no solution is completely satisfactory: for example, in British English (RP) the word 'hurry' could be divided /ˈhʌ.ri/ or /ˈhʌr.i/, but the former would result in an analysis with a syllable-final /ʌ/ (which is held to be non-occurring) while the latter would result in a syllable final /r/ (which is said not to occur in this accent). Some phonologists have suggested a compromise analysis where the consonant in the middle belongs to both syllables, resulting in an analysis of 'hurry' which comprises the syllables /hʌr/ and/ri/, and the medial /r/ is described as ambisyllabic.
Where the division coincides with a word boundary, or the boundary between elements of a compound word, it is not usual to insist on the maximal onsets principle in a way that divides words in a counter-intuitive way; thus the word 'hardware' would be divided /ˈhɑː.dweə/ by the M.O.P., but dictionaries prefer the division /ˈhɑːd.weə/. For discussion of this topic, see Gimson,[38] Giegerich[39] or Kreidler[40]
In the approach used by the Longman Pronunciation Dictionary, Wells[30] claims that consonants syllabify with the preceding rather than following vowel when the preceding vowel is the nucleus of a more salient syllable, with stressed syllables being the most salient, reduced syllables the least, and full unstressed vowels ("secondary stress") intermediate. But there are lexical differences as well, frequently but not exclusively with compound words. For example, in dolphin and selfish, Wells argues that the stressed syllable ends in /lf/, but in shellfish, the /f/ belongs with the following syllable: /ˈdɒlf.ɪn/, /ˈsɛlf.ɪʃ/ → [ˈdɒlfɨn], [ˈsɛlfɨʃ], but /ˈʃɛl.fɪʃ/ → [ˈʃɛlˑfɪʃ], where the /l/ is a little longer and the /ɪ/ is not reduced. Similarly, in toe-strap Wells argues that the second /t/ is a full plosive, as usual in syllable onset, whereas in toast-rack the second /t/ is in many dialects reduced to the unreleased allophone it takes in syllable codas, or even elided: /ˈtoʊ.stræp/, /ˈtoʊst.ræk/ → [ˈtʰoˑʊstɹæp], [ˈtoʊs(t̚)ɹʷæk]; likewise nitrate /ˈnaɪ.treɪt/ → [ˈnʌɪtɹ̥ʷeɪt] with a voiceless /r/ (and for some people an affricated tr as in tree), vs night-rate /ˈnaɪt.reɪt/ → [ˈnʌɪt̚ɹʷeɪt] with a voiced /r/. Cues of syllable boundaries include aspiration of syllable onsets and (in the US) flapping of coda /t, d/ (a tease /ə.ˈtiːz/ → [əˈtʰiːz] vs. at ease /æt.ˈiːz/ → [æɾˈiːz]), epenthetic stops like [t] in syllable codas (fence /ˈfɛns/ → [ˈfɛnts]but inside /ɪn.ˈsaɪd/ → [ɪnˈsaɪd]), and r-colored vowels when the /r/ is in the coda vs. labialization when it is in the onset (key-ring /ˈkiː.rɪŋ/ → [ˈkʰiːɹʷɪŋ] but fearing /ˈfiːr.ɪŋ/ →[ˈfɪəɹɪŋ]).
[edit]Onset
The following can occur as the onset:
All single consonant phonemes except /ŋ/

Stop plus approximant other than /j/:
/pl/, /bl/, /kl/, /ɡl/, /pr/, /br/, /tr/,[1] /dr/,[1] /kr/, /ɡr/, /tw/, /dw/, /ɡw/,/kw/, /pw/
play, blood, clean, glove, prize, bring, tree,[1] dream,[1] crowd, green, twin, dwarf, language, quick, puissance
Voiceless fricative plus approximant other than /j/:[2]
/fl/, /sl/, /θl/,[3] /fr/, /θr/, /ʃr/, /hw/,[4] /sw/, /θw/, /vw/
floor, sleep, thlipsis,[3] friend, three, shrimp, what,[4] swing, thwart, reservoir
Consonant plus /j/ (before /uː/ or /ʊr/):
/pj/, /bj/, /tj/,[5] /dj/,[5] /kj/, /ɡj/, /mj/, /nj/,[5] /fj/, /vj/, /θj/,[5] /sj/,[5]/zj/,[5] /hj/, /lj/[5]
pure, beautiful, tube,[5] during,[5] cute, argue, music, new,[5] few, view, thew,[5] suit,[5]Zeus,[5] huge, lurid[5]
/s/ plus voiceless stop:[6]
/sp/, /st/, /sk/
speak, stop, skill
/s/ plus nasal other than /ŋ/:[6]
/sm/, /sn/
smile, snow
/s/ plus voiceless fricative:[3]
/sf/, /sθ/
sphere, sthenic
/s/ plus voiceless stop plus approximant:[6]
/spl/, /skl/,[3] /spr/, /str/, /skr/, /skw/, /smj/, /spj/, /stj/,[5] /skj/
split, sclera, spring, street, scream, square, smew, spew, student,[5] skewer
/s/ plus voiceless fricative plus approximant:[3]
/sfr/
sphragistics
Notes:
1.      For a number of speakers, /tr/ and /dr/ tend to affricate, so that tree resembles "chree", and dream resembles "jream".[41][42][43] This is sometimes transcribed as [tʃr] and[dʒr] respectively, but the pronunciation varies and may, for example, be closer to [tʂ] and [dʐ][44] or with a fricative release similar in quality to the rhotic, i.e. [tɹ̝̊ɹ̥], [dɹ̝ɹ], or[tʂɻ], [dʐɻ].
2.      In some dialects[which?], /wr/ (rather than /r/) occurs in words beginning in wr- (write, wrong, wren, etc.).[citation needed]
3.      Words beginning in unusual consonant clusters that originated in Latinized Greek loanwords tend to drop the first phoneme, as in */bd/, */fθ/, */ɡn/, */hr/, */kn/, */ks/, */kt/, */kθ/, */mn/, */pn/, */ps/, */pt/, */tm/, and */θm/, which have become /d/ (bdellium), /θ/ (phthisis), /n/ (gnome), /r/ (rhythm), /n/ (cnidoblast), /z/(xylophone), /t/ (ctenophore), /θ/ (chthonic), /n/ (mnemonic), /n/ (pneumonia), /s/ (psychology), /t/ (pterodactyl), /m/ (tmesis), and /m/ (asthma). However, the onsets/sf/, /sfr/, /skl/, /sθ/, and /θl/ have remained intact.
4.      The onset /hw/ is simplified to /w/ in many dialects (wine–whine merger).
5.      There is an on-going sound change (yod-dropping) by which /j/ as the final consonant in a cluster is being lost. In RP, words with /sj/ and /lj/ can usually be pronounced with or without this sound, e.g., [suːt] or [sjuːt]. For some speakers of English, including some British speakers, the sound change is more advanced and so, for example,General American does not contain the onsets /tj/, /dj/, /nj/, /θj/, /sj/, /stj/, /zj/, or /lj/. Words that would otherwise begin in these onsets drop the /j/: e.g., tube (/tuːb/), during (/ˈdʊrɪŋ/), new (/nuː/), Thule (/ˈθuːliː/), suit (/suːt/), student (/ˈstuːdənt/), Zeus (/zuːs/), lurid (/ˈlʊrɪd/). In some dialects, such Welsh English, /j/may occur in more combinations; for example in /tʃj/ (chew), /dʒj/ (Jew), /ʃj/ (sure), and /slj/ (slew).
6.      Many clusters beginning with /ʃ/ and paralleling native clusters beginning with /s/ are found initially in German and Yiddish loanwords, such as /ʃl/, /ʃp/, /ʃt/, /ʃm/, /ʃn/, /ʃpr/, /ʃtr/ (in words such as schlep, spiel, shtick, schmuck, schnapps, Shprintzen's, strudel). /ʃw/ is found initially in the Hebrew loanword schwa. Before /r/however, the native cluster is /ʃr/. The opposite cluster /sr/ is found in loanwords such as Sri Lanka, but this can be nativized by changing it to /ʃr/.
Other onsets
Certain English onsets appear only in contractions: e.g., /zbl/ ('sblood), and /zw/ or /dzw/ ('swounds or 'dswounds). Some, such as /pʃ/ (pshaw), /fw/ (fwoosh), or /vr/(vroom), can occur in interjections. An archaic voiceless fricative plus nasal exists, /fn/ (fnese), as does an archaic /snj/ (snew).
A few other onsets occur in further (anglicized) loan words, including /bw/ (bwana), /mw/ (moiré), /nw/ (noire), /zw/ (zwieback), /kv/ (kvetch), /ʃv/ (schvartze), /tv/ (Tver),/vl/ (Vladimir), and /zl/ (zloty).
Some clusters of this type can be converted to regular English phonotactics by simplifying the cluster: e.g. /(d)z/ (dziggetai), /(h)r/ (Hrolf), /kr(w)/ (croissant), /(p)f/ (pfennig),/(f)θ/ (phthalic), and /(t)s/ (tsunami).
Others can be substituted by native clusters differing only in voice: /zb ~ sp/ (sbirro), and /zɡr ~ skr/ (sgraffito).
[edit]Nucleus
The following can occur as the nucleus:
·         All vowel sounds
·         /m/, /n/ and /l/ in certain situations (see below under word-level rules)
·         /r/ in rhotic varieties of English (e.g. General American) in certain situations (see below under word-level rules)
[edit]Coda
Most (in theory, all) the following except those that end with /s/, /z/, /ʃ/, /ʒ/, /tʃ/ or /dʒ/ can be extended with /s/ or /z/ representing the morpheme -s/z-. Similarly, most (in theory, all) the following except those that end with /t/ or /d/ can be extended with /t/ or /d/ representing the morpheme -t/d-.
Wells (1990) argues that a variety of syllable codas are possible in English, even /ntr, ndr/ in words like entry /ˈɛntr.ɪ/ and sundry /ˈsʌndr.ɪ/, with /tr, dr/ being treated as affricates along the lines of /tʃ, dʒ/. He argues that the traditional assumption that pre-vocalic consonants form a syllable with the following vowel is due to the influence of languages like French and Latin, where syllable structure is CVC.CVC regardless of stress placement. Disregarding such contentious cases, which do not occur at the ends of words, the following sequences can occur as the coda:
The single consonant phonemes except /h/, /w/, /j/ and, in non-rhotic varieties, /r/

Lateral approximant plus stop or affricate: /lp/, /lb/, /lt/, /ld/, /ltʃ/, /ldʒ/, /lk/
help, bulb, belt, hold, belch, indulge, milk
In rhotic varieties, /r/ plus stop or affricate: /rp/, /rb/, /rt/, /rd/, /rtʃ/, /rdʒ/, /rk/, /rɡ/
harp, orb, fort, beard, arch, large, mark, morgue
Lateral approximant + fricative: /lf/, /lv/, /lθ/, /ls/, /lʃ/
golf, solve, wealth, else, Welsh
In rhotic varieties, /r/ + fricative: /rf/, /rv/, /rθ/, /rs/, /rz/, /rʃ/
dwarf, carve, north, force, Mars, marsh
Lateral approximant + nasal: /lm/, /ln/
film, kiln
In rhotic varieties, /r/ + nasal or lateral: /rm/, /rn/, /rl/
arm, born, snarl
Nasal + homorganic stop or affricate: /mp/, /nt/, /nd/, /ntʃ/, /ndʒ/, /ŋk/
jump, tent, end, lunch, lounge, pink
Nasal + fricative: /mf/, /mθ/, /nθ/, /ns/, /nz/, /ŋθ/ in some varieties
triumph, gloomth, month, prince, bronze, length
Voiceless fricative plus voiceless stop: /ft/, /sp/, /st/, /sk/
left, crisp, lost, ask
Two voiceless fricatives: /fθ/
fifth
Two voiceless stops: /pt/, /kt/
opt, act
Stop plus voiceless fricative: /pθ/, /ps/, /tθ/, /ts/, /dθ/, /ks/
depth, lapse, eighth, klutz, width, box
Lateral approximant + two consonants: /lpt/, /lfθ/, /lts/, /lst/, /lkt/, /lks/
sculpt, twelfth, waltz, whilst, mulct, calx
In rhotic varieties, /r/ + two consonants: /rmθ/, /rpt/, /rps/, /rts/, /rst/, /rkt/
warmth, excerpt, corpse, quartz, horst, infarct
Nasal + homorganic stop + stop or fricative: /mpt/, /mps/, /ndθ/, /ŋkt/, /ŋks/, /ŋkθ/ in some varieties
prompt, glimpse, thousandth, distinct, jinx, length
Three obstruents: /ksθ/, /kst/
sixth, next
Note: For some speakers, a fricative before /θ/ is elided so that these never appear phonetically: /ˈfɪfθ/ becomes [ˈfɪθ], /ˈsiksθ/ becomes [ˈsikθ], /ˈtwɛlfθ/ becomes [ˈtwɛlθ].
[edit]Syllable-level rules
·         Both the onset and the coda are optional
·         /j/ at the end of an onset cluster (/pj/, /bj/, /tj/, /dj/, /kj/, /fj/, /vj/, /θj/, /sj/, /zj/, /hj/, /mj/, /nj/, /lj/, /spj/, /stj/, /skj/) must be followed by /uː/ or /ʊə/
·         Long vowels and diphthongs are not found before /ŋ/ except for the mimetic word boing![45]
·         /ʊ/ is rare in syllable-initial position[46] (although, in the northern half of England, [ʊ] is used for /ʌ/ and is common at the start of syllables).
·         Stop + /w/ before /uː, ʊ, ʌ, aʊ/ (all presently or historically /u(ː)/) are excluded[47]
·         Sequences of /s/ + C1 +  + C1, where C1 is a consonant other than /t/ and  is a short vowel, are virtually nonexistent[47]
[edit]Word-level rules
·         /ə/ does not occur in stressed syllables
·         /ʒ/ does not occur in word-initial position in native English words although it can occur syllable-initial, e.g., luxurious /lʌɡˈʒʊəriəs/
·         /m/, /n/, /l/ and, in rhotic varieties, /r/ can be the syllable nucleus (i.e. a syllabic consonant) in an unstressed syllable following another consonant, especially /t/, /d/, /s/or /z/
·         Certain short vowel sounds, called checked vowels, cannot occur without a coda in a single-syllable word. In RP, the following short vowel sounds are checked: /ɪ/, /ɛ/, /æ/, /ɒ/, /ʌ/, and /ʊ/.
[edit]Prosody
The prosodic features of English – stress, rhythm, and intonation – can be described as follows.
[edit]Prosodic stress
Prosodic stress is extra stress given to words or syllables when they appear in certain positions in an utterance, or when they receive special emphasis.
According to Ladefoged's analysis (as referred to under Lexical stress above), English normally has prosodic stress on the final stressed syllable in an intonation unit. This is said to be the origin of the distinction traditionally made at the lexical level between primary and secondary stress: when a word like admiration (traditionally transcribed as something like /ˌædmɨˈreɪʃən/) is spoken in isolation, or at the end of a sentence, the syllable ra (the final stressed syllable) is pronounced with greater force than the syllable ad, although when the word is not pronounced with this final intonation there may be no difference between the levels of stress of these two syllables.
Prosodic stress can shift for various pragmatic functions, such as focus or contrast. For instance, in the dialogue Is it brunch tomorrow? No, it's dinner tomorrow, the extra stress shifts from the last stressed syllable of the sentence, tomorrow, to the last stressed syllable of the emphasized word, dinner.
Grammatical function words are usually prosodically unstressed, although they can acquire stress when emphasized (as in Did you find the cat? Well, I found a cat). Many English function words have distinct strong and weak pronunciations; for example, the word a in the last example is pronounced /eɪ/, while the more common unstressed a is pronounced /ə/. See Weak and strong forms in English.
[edit]Rhythm
English is a stress-timed language. That is, stressed syllables appear at a roughly steady tempo, and non-stressed syllables are shortened to accommodate this.
[edit]Intonation
English declarative sentences generally have a pattern of rising pitch on the final stressed syllable followed by falling pitch on the subsequent unstressed syllables (or on the last part of the final stressed syllable itself, if it is also the last syllable of the sentence). But if something is left unsaid, the final fall in pitch occurs only to a lesser extent. Wh-questions, and tag questions with declarative intent, follow the same pattern as do declarative sentences.
In contrast, yes-no questions show pitch rising on the last stressed syllable, and remaining high on any subsequent syllables.
[edit]History of English pronunciation
English consonants have been remarkably stable over time, and have undergone few changes in the last 1500 years. On the other hand, English vowels have been quite unstable. Not surprisingly, then, the main differences between modern dialects almost always involve vowels.
Around the late 14th century, English began to undergo the Great Vowel Shift, in which
·         The high long vowels [iː] and [uː] in words like price and mouth became diphthongized, first to [əɪ] and [əʊ] (where they remain today in some environments in some accents such as Canadian English) and later to their modern values [aɪ] and [aʊ]. This is not unique to English, as this also happened in Dutch (first shift only) and German (both shifts).
·         The other long vowels became higher:
·      [eː] became [iː] (for example meet).
·      [aː] became [eː] (later diphthongized to [eɪ], for example name).
·      [oː] became [uː] (for example goose).
·      [ɔː] become [oː] (later diphthongized to [əʊ] (RP) and [oʊ] (GA), for example bone).
Later developments complicate the picture: whereas in Geoffrey Chaucer's time foodgood, and blood all had the vowel [oː] and in William Shakespeare's time they all had the vowel [uː], in modern pronunciation good has shortened its vowel to [ʊ] and blood has shortened and lowered its vowel to [ʌ] in most accents. In Shakespeare's day (late 16th-early 17th century),[48] many rhymes were possible that no longer hold today.[49] For example, in his play The Taming of the Shrewshrew rhymed with woe.[50]
[edit]Dialectal differences
[edit]æ-tensing
æ-tensing is a phenomenon found in many varieties of American English by which the vowel /æ/ has a longer, higher, and usually diphthongal pronunciation in some environments, usually to something like [eə]. Some American accents, for example those of New York City, Philadelphia, and Baltimore, make a marginal phonemic distinction between /æ/ and/eə/ although the two occur largely in mutually exclusive environments.
[edit]Bad–lad split
The bad–lad split refers to the situation in some varieties of southern British English and Australian English, where a long phoneme /æː/ in words like bad contrasts with a short /æ/ in words like lad.
[edit]Cot–caught merger
The cot–caught merger is a sound change by which the vowel of words like caughttalk, and tall (/ɔː/), is pronounced the same as the vowel of words like cotrock, and doll (/ɒ/in New England /ɑː/ elsewhere). This merger is widespread in North American English, being found in approximately 40% of American speakers and virtually all Canadianspeakers.
[edit]Father–bother merger
The father–bother merger is the pronunciation of the short O /ɒ/ in words such as "bother" identically to the broad A /ɑː/ of words such as "father", nearly universal in all of the United States and Canada save New England and the Maritime provinces; many American dictionaries use the same symbol for these vowels in pronunciation guides.